Context and data played a role in eliciting some misconceptions. For e การแปล - Context and data played a role in eliciting some misconceptions. For e อังกฤษ วิธีการพูด

Context and data played a role in e

Context and data played a role in eliciting some misconceptions. For example, consider the
results for the four main misconceptions. The use of chance cannot be measured mathematically
does not seem to depend on data or context. The compound approach is influenced by data but not
influenced by context. The approach was used more often when the compositions of bags/spinners
were quite different from each other than they are equal or close. Actually, when the compositions
were quite different from each other, the results from using the compound approach are in
agreement with those that could be deduced from probability calculations. The outcome approach is
influenced by context but may not be influenced by data (whether using a 50% chance elicits more
outcome approach responses is still unclear). Both context and data affect the equiprobability bias.
More equiprobable responses were observed in drawing names’ items than drawing marbles or
spinning arrowhead items, and also more equiprobable responses were observed when the
compositions were close than when they were far apart. It may be that students' familiarity with
drawing a name, where everyone has an equal chance (all names are equiprobable) leads to more
equiprobability misconceptions. When the data are close some students use the specific version of
equiprobability, namely that if the chances are close they are the same in practice, resulting in more
equiprobability responses.
0/5000
จาก: -
เป็น: -
ผลลัพธ์ (อังกฤษ) 1: [สำเนา]
คัดลอก!
Context and data played a role in eliciting some misconceptions. For example, consider the
results for the four main misconceptions. The use of chance cannot be measured mathematically
does not seem to depend on data or context. The compound approach is influenced by data but not
influenced by context. The approach was used more often when the compositions of bags/spinners
were quite different from each other than they are equal or close. Actually, when the compositions
were quite different from each other, the results from using the compound approach are in
agreement with those that could be deduced from probability calculations. The outcome approach is
influenced by context but may not be influenced by data (whether using a 50% chance elicits more
outcome approach responses is still unclear). Both context and data affect the equiprobability bias.
More equiprobable responses were observed in drawing names' items than drawing marbles or
spinning arrowhead items, and also more equiprobable responses were observed when the
compositions were close than when they were far apart. It may be that students' familiarity with
drawing a name, where everyone has an equal chance (all names are equiprobable) leads to more
equiprobability misconceptions. When the data are close some students use the specific version of
equiprobability, namely that if the chances are close they are the same in practice, resulting in more
equiprobability responses.
การแปล กรุณารอสักครู่..
ผลลัพธ์ (อังกฤษ) 2:[สำเนา]
คัดลอก!
Context and data played a role in eliciting some misconceptions. For example, consider the
results for the four main misconceptions. The use of chance cannot be measured mathematically
does not seem to depend on data or context. The compound approach is influenced by data but not
influenced by context. The approach was used more often when the compositions of bags/spinners
were quite different from each other than they are equal or close. Actually, when the compositions
were quite different from each other, the results from using the compound approach are in
agreement with those that could be deduced from probability calculations. The outcome approach is
influenced by context but may not be influenced by data (whether using a 50% chance elicits more
outcome approach responses is still unclear). Both context and data affect the equiprobability bias.
More equiprobable responses were observed in drawing names’ items than drawing marbles or
spinning arrowhead items, and also more equiprobable responses were observed when the
compositions were close than when they were far apart. It may be that students' familiarity with
drawing a name, where everyone has an equal chance (all names are equiprobable) leads to more
equiprobability misconceptions. When the data are close some students use the specific version of
equiprobability, namely that if the chances are close they are the same in practice, resulting in more
equiprobability responses.
การแปล กรุณารอสักครู่..
ผลลัพธ์ (อังกฤษ) 3:[สำเนา]
คัดลอก!
Context and data played a role in eliciting some misconceptions. For, example consider the
results for the four main, misconceptions The use of chance cannot be measured mathematically
does not seem to depend on data or context. The compound approach is Influenced by data but not
influenced by context. The approach was used more often when the compositions of bags / spinners
Were quite different from each other than they are equal or close. Actually when the, compositions
were quite different From each other the results, from using the compound approach are in
agreement with those that could be deduced from probability Calculations. The outcome approach is
influenced by context but may not be influenced by data (whether using a 50% chance Elicits more
Outcome approach responses is still unclear). Both context and data affect the equiprobability bias.
More equiprobable Responses were observed in drawing names' items than drawing marbles or
spinning arrowhead items and also, more equiprobable Responses were observed when the
compositions were close than when they were far apart. It may be that students familiarity. ' With
drawing, a nameWhere everyone has an equal chance (all names are equiprobable) leads to more
equiprobability misconceptions. When the Data are close some students use the specific version of
equiprobability namely that, if the chances are close they are The same in practice resulting in, more
equiprobability responses
การแปล กรุณารอสักครู่..
 
ภาษาอื่น ๆ
การสนับสนุนเครื่องมือแปลภาษา: กรีก, กันนาดา, กาลิเชียน, คลิงออน, คอร์สิกา, คาซัค, คาตาลัน, คินยารวันดา, คีร์กิซ, คุชราต, จอร์เจีย, จีน, จีนดั้งเดิม, ชวา, ชิเชวา, ซามัว, ซีบัวโน, ซุนดา, ซูลู, ญี่ปุ่น, ดัตช์, ตรวจหาภาษา, ตุรกี, ทมิฬ, ทาจิก, ทาทาร์, นอร์เวย์, บอสเนีย, บัลแกเรีย, บาสก์, ปัญจาป, ฝรั่งเศส, พาชตู, ฟริเชียน, ฟินแลนด์, ฟิลิปปินส์, ภาษาอินโดนีเซี, มองโกเลีย, มัลทีส, มาซีโดเนีย, มาราฐี, มาลากาซี, มาลายาลัม, มาเลย์, ม้ง, ยิดดิช, ยูเครน, รัสเซีย, ละติน, ลักเซมเบิร์ก, ลัตเวีย, ลาว, ลิทัวเนีย, สวาฮิลี, สวีเดน, สิงหล, สินธี, สเปน, สโลวัก, สโลวีเนีย, อังกฤษ, อัมฮาริก, อาร์เซอร์ไบจัน, อาร์เมเนีย, อาหรับ, อิกโบ, อิตาลี, อุยกูร์, อุสเบกิสถาน, อูรดู, ฮังการี, ฮัวซา, ฮาวาย, ฮินดี, ฮีบรู, เกลิกสกอต, เกาหลี, เขมร, เคิร์ด, เช็ก, เซอร์เบียน, เซโซโท, เดนมาร์ก, เตลูกู, เติร์กเมน, เนปาล, เบงกอล, เบลารุส, เปอร์เซีย, เมารี, เมียนมา (พม่า), เยอรมัน, เวลส์, เวียดนาม, เอสเปอแรนโต, เอสโทเนีย, เฮติครีโอล, แอฟริกา, แอลเบเนีย, โคซา, โครเอเชีย, โชนา, โซมาลี, โปรตุเกส, โปแลนด์, โยรูบา, โรมาเนีย, โอเดีย (โอริยา), ไทย, ไอซ์แลนด์, ไอร์แลนด์, การแปลภาษา.

Copyright ©2024 I Love Translation. All reserved.

E-mail: